由买买提看人间百态

boards

本页内容为未名空间相应帖子的节选和存档,一周内的贴子最多显示50字,超过一周显示500字 访问原贴
Military版 - 科研造假,低素质是个大问题
相关主题
发现投scientific reports这种期刊非常不划算一个情景说明英语的脑残
中国人之奴性 ZT为什么非要把不正常的东西让人家承认是正常的。
It's not possible to repeat something 40 years ago生物进化
果黑评Jobs《自然》杂志:报道不应作为韩春雨实验可重复的证据
Naomi Wolf: 外在美是压迫女性的工具!!终于出来第一步Nature Biotech: editorial expression of concer (转载)
世界上菜吃了一圈,发现一个饮食公理新华社评韩春雨撤论文:用科学态度对待科学问题
谷歌不敢把围棋机器人拿出来肯定有猫腻为什么种族将会使美国分裂 (转载)
李大师当年被Time采访,很经典“男人为什么会存在”长期以来一直困扰着科学家们
相关话题的讨论汇总
话题: begley话题: cancer话题: amgen话题: studies
进入Military版参与讨论
1 (共1页)
l*****7
发帖数: 8463
1
科研造假,低素质是个大问题
1.
http://www.reuters.com/article/2012/03/28/us-science-cancer-idU
In cancer science, many "discoveries" don't hold up
2.
http://www.mitbbs.com/article_t/Biology/31915105.html
eLife将检验50篇高引用率文章
发信人: Addgene (基因加加加), 信区: Biology
标 题: eLife将检验50篇高引用率文章
发信站: BBS 未名空间站 (Mon Sep 22 16:30:07 2014, 美东)
ELife 编辑Randy Schekman 在访谈中说:“We've been approached by an
organization called the Reproducibility Project, where a private foundation
has agreed to provide funds for experiments in the fifty most highly cited
papers in cancer biology to be reproduced, and the work will be contracted
out to laboratories for replication. And we've agreed to handle this and
eventually to publish the replication studies, so we'll see, you know, at
least with these fifty papers. How many of them really have reproducibility.
The reproducibility studies will be published in eLife. We're just getting
going in that, so it may be a couple of years, but that's what we'd like to
do.”
链接:
http://metode.cat/en/Issues/Interview/Randy-Schekman
l*****7
发帖数: 8463
2
http://www.reuters.com/article/2012/03/28/us-science-cancer-idU
(Reuters) - A former researcher at Amgen Inc has found that many basic
studies on cancer -- a high proportion of them from university labs -- are
unreliable, with grim consequences for producing new medicines in the future.
During a decade as head of global cancer research at Amgen, C. Glenn Begley
identified 53 "landmark" publications -- papers in top journals, from
reputable labs -- for his team to reproduce. Begley sought to double-check
the findings before trying to build on them for drug development.
Result: 47 of the 53 could not be replicated. He described his findings in a
commentary piece published on Wednesday in the journal Nature.
"It was shocking," said Begley, now senior vice president of privately held
biotechnology company TetraLogic, which develops cancer drugs. "These are
the studies the pharmaceutical industry relies on to identify new targets
for drug development. But if you're going to place a $1 million or $2
million or $5 million bet on an observation, you need to be sure it's true.
As we tried to reproduce these papers we became convinced you can't take
anything at face value."
The failure to win "the war on cancer" has been blamed on many factors, from
the use of mouse models that are irrelevant to human cancers to risk-averse
funding agencies. But recently a new culprit has emerged: too many basic
scientific discoveries, done in animals or cells growing in lab dishes and
meant to show the way to a new drug, are wrong.
Begley's experience echoes a report from scientists at Bayer AG last year.
Neither group of researchers alleges fraud, nor would they identify the
research they had tried to replicate.
But they and others fear the phenomenon is the product of a skewed system of
incentives that has academics cutting corners to further their careers.
George Robertson of Dalhousie University in Nova Scotia previously worked at
Merck on neurodegenerative diseases such as Parkinson's. While at Merck, he
also found many academic studies that did not hold up.
"It drives people in industry crazy. Why are we seeing a collapse of the
pharma and biotech industries? One possibility is that academia is not
providing accurate findings," he said.
BELIEVE IT OR NOT
Over the last two decades, the most promising route to new cancer drugs has
been one pioneered by the discoverers of Gleevec, the Novartis drug that
targets a form of leukemia, and Herceptin, Genentech's breast-cancer drug.
In each case, scientists discovered a genetic change that turned a normal
cell into a malignant one. Those findings allowed them to develop a molecule
that blocks the cancer-producing process.
This approach led to an explosion of claims of other potential "druggable"
targets. Amgen tried to replicate the new papers before launching its own
drug-discovery projects.
Scientists at Bayer did not have much more success. In a 2011 paper titled,
"Believe it or not," they analyzed in-house projects that built on "exciting
published data" from basic science studies. "Often, key data could not be
reproduced," wrote Khusru Asadullah, vice president and head of target
discovery at Bayer HealthCare in Berlin, and colleagues.
Of 47 cancer projects at Bayer during 2011, less than one-quarter could
reproduce previously reported findings, despite the efforts of three or four
scientists working full time for up to a year. Bayer dropped the projects.
Bayer and Amgen found that the prestige of a journal was no guarantee a
paper would be solid. "The scientific community assumes that the claims in a
preclinical study can be taken at face value," Begley and Lee Ellis of MD
Anderson Cancer Center wrote in Nature. It assumes, too, that "the main
message of the paper can be relied on ... Unfortunately, this is not always
the case."
When the Amgen replication team of about 100 scientists could not confirm
reported results, they contacted the authors. Those who cooperated discussed
what might account for the inability of Amgen to confirm the results. Some
let Amgen borrow antibodies and other materials used in the original study
or even repeat experiments under the original authors' direction.
Some authors required the Amgen scientists sign a confidentiality agreement
barring them from disclosing data at odds with the original findings. "The
world will never know" which 47 studies -- many of them highly cited -- are
apparently wrong, Begley said.
The most common response by the challenged scientists was: "you didn't do it
right." Indeed, cancer biology is fiendishly complex, noted Phil Sharp, a
cancer biologist and Nobel laureate at the Massachusetts Institute of
Technology.
Even in the most rigorous studies, the results might be reproducible only in
very specific conditions, Sharp explained: "A cancer cell might respond one
way in one set of conditions and another way in different conditions. I
think a lot of the variability can come from that."
THE BEST STORY
Other scientists worry that something less innocuous explains the lack of
reproducibility.
Part way through his project to reproduce promising studies, Begley met for
breakfast at a cancer conference with the lead scientist of one of the
problematic studies.
"We went through the paper line by line, figure by figure," said Begley. "I
explained that we re-did their experiment 50 times and never got their
result. He said they'd done it six times and got this result once, but put
it in the paper because it made the best story. It's very disillusioning."
Such selective publication is just one reason the scientific literature is
peppered with incorrect results.
For one thing, basic science studies are rarely "blinded" the way clinical
trials are. That is, researchers know which cell line or mouse got a
treatment or had cancer. That can be a problem when data are subject to
interpretation, as a researcher who is intellectually invested in a theory
is more likely to interpret ambiguous evidence in its favor.
The problem goes beyond cancer.
On Tuesday, a committee of the National Academy of Sciences heard testimony
that the number of scientific papers that had to be retracted increased more
than tenfold over the last decade; the number of journal articles published
rose only 44 percent.
Ferric Fang of the University of Washington, speaking to the panel, said he
blamed a hypercompetitive academic environment that fosters poor science and
even fraud, as too many researchers compete for diminishing funding.
"The surest ticket to getting a grant or job is getting published in a high-
profile journal," said Fang. "This is an unhealthy belief that can lead a
scientist to engage in sensationalism and sometimes even dishonest behavior."
The academic reward system discourages efforts to ensure a finding was not a
fluke. Nor is there an incentive to verify someone else's discovery. As
recently as the late 1990s, most potential cancer-drug targets were backed
by 100 to 200 publications. Now each may have fewer than half a dozen.
"If you can write it up and get it published you're not even thinking of
reproducibility," said Ken Kaitin, director of the Tufts Center for the
Study of Drug Development. "You make an observation and move on. There is no
incentive to find out it was wrong."
(Note: Amgen researcher C. Glenn Begley is not related to the author of this
story, Sharon Begley)
(Reporting By Sharon Begley; Editing by Michele Gershberg and Maureen Bavdek
)
1 (共1页)
进入Military版参与讨论
相关主题
“男人为什么会存在”长期以来一直困扰着科学家们Naomi Wolf: 外在美是压迫女性的工具!!
基因编辑孩子问题上西方伦理的concern来自几点世界上菜吃了一圈,发现一个饮食公理
脑门贴张纸,骗过最强人脸识别系统!华为莫斯科研究院出品,FaceID已阵亡谷歌不敢把围棋机器人拿出来肯定有猫腻
Nieng的文章没啥看头李大师当年被Time采访,很经典
发现投scientific reports这种期刊非常不划算一个情景说明英语的脑残
中国人之奴性 ZT为什么非要把不正常的东西让人家承认是正常的。
It's not possible to repeat something 40 years ago生物进化
果黑评Jobs《自然》杂志:报道不应作为韩春雨实验可重复的证据
相关话题的讨论汇总
话题: begley话题: cancer话题: amgen话题: studies